Skip to main content
Displaying 1 of 1
Drug use for grown-ups : chasing liberty in the land of fear
2021
Where is it?
First Chapter or Excerpt
I am an unapologetic drug user. I take drugs as part of my pursuit of happiness, and they work. I am a happier and better person because of them. I am also a scientist and a professor of psychology specializing in neuroscience at Columbia University, known for my work on drug abuse and addiction. It has taken me more than two decades to come out of the closet about my personal drug use. Simply put, I have been a coward. The philosopher John Locke once noted that pursuing happiness is "the foundation of liberty." This idea is at the core of the Declaration of Independence, the document that gave birth to our nation. The Declaration asserts that each of us is endowed with certain "unalienable Rights," including "Life, Liberty and the pursuit of Happiness," and that governments are created for the purpose of protecting these rights. The use of drugs in the pursuit of happiness, in my view, is arguably an act that the government is obliged to safeguard. Why is our government arresting hundreds of thousands of Americans each year for using drugs, for pursuing pleasure, for seeking happiness? The short answer is that it's a very long story. The long answer is the book you are reading. America's drug regime is a monstrous, incoherent mess. TO GRASP HOW we got here and what we can do about it, I'd like to start by telling you something about my life and work as a "drug abuse" scientist. In the fall of 1999, I landed my dream job, as an assistant professor and researcher at Columbia University's College of Physicians and Surgeons. My research involved giving thousands of doses of drugs, including crack cocaine, marijuana, and metham- phetamine, to a range of people in order to study the effects. I believed my work contributed to our understanding of drug addiction. I would be awarded multimillion-dollar grants from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) to conduct this research, and I would be invited to serve on some of the most prestigious committees in the area of neuropsychopharmacology. It was a thrilling time. Twenty years later--twenty years I've spent studying the inter- actions among the brain, drugs, and behavior and observing how moralizing about drug use is expressed in social policy--my initial excitement has given way to skepticism, cynicism, and disillusionment. When I was a naive graduate student, I believed that I was doing God's work by telling people to stay away from drugs. I believed that the poverty and crime that plagued my childhood com- munity were a direct result of drug use and addiction. I now know that telling people to avoid drugs is no more godly than the Church prohibiting my Catholic wife from using birth control, but it is just as paternalistic, a way of restricting one's freedom and autonomy. What about the notion that drugs led to poverty and crime in my neighborhood? Well, that is simply an ugly fantasy, an incredibly effective one to be sure. It's effective not only because it is still believed by large segments of the American public but also because it seemingly provides a simple solution to complicated problems faced by poor and desperate people. Many other complex factors are responsible for the turmoil seen in the places of my youth and other communities. But it took me a long time to see that clearly myself. I was too busy for too long being a soldier in the regime, caught up in the cause of "proving" how dangerous drug use is. I HAD THE COOLEST job in the world. I got people high on a daily basis. I instructed the twenty-five-year-old white man to light the marijuana cigarette, which was to be smoked through a hollow plastic cigarette holder so that the contents were not visible. He inhaled for five seconds, then held the smoke in his lungs for another ten seconds before exhaling. He repeated this two more times, with a forty-second interval between each puff. We called this our paced- puffing procedure. We used it to standardize, to the best of our ability, the amount of drug inhaled. Although I couldn't know for sure whether he was getting placebo or active THC, the major psychoactive ingredient in marijuana, I could tell from his glassy red eyes and the serene smile on his boyish flushed face that he really enjoyed what he had gotten. Nodding slowly and with more bass in his voice than usual, he said, "Yeah, that's it." I could also tell that he was an experienced smoker; it took him only three puffs to suck down nearly three-fourths of the 1 g cigarette. Marijuana smoke now filled the small sterile room. The smoker, whom I'll call John, was a research participant in one of my studies. And there I was, a young black dreadlocked scientist, trying to conceal the perpetual anxiety I felt about having the strong, distinct smell of marijuana in my hair for another entire workday. I was concerned that as I traveled, on the elevator, from floor to floor or sat in on a lecture or meeting, some judgmental per- son might think disparagingly, "Typical, dreadlock smoking while at work." Never mind that marijuana has never been my primary drug of choice. Never mind that I had a personal rule, for fear of biasing my results, against using the drug I was currently studying. The year was 2000. In this particular experiment, I was trying to understand how cannabis affected regular users' brain functioning and behavior. I had received a grant from NIDA to conduct the study. My hard work and commitment were finally paying off. When I started the study, I believed, as did most people, that pot temporarily impairs mental processes such that smokers exhibit memory problems and other cognitive disruptions. There are certainly plenty of anecdotal accounts in line with that view. But, of course, anecdote is not evidence. That's why we do the science. Still, there are even scientific data suggesting that pot temporarily diminishes short-term memory ability in infrequent users. Of course, this is not surprising, because many drugs--alcohol, Ambien, and Xanax, among others-- temporarily disrupt selective mental processes in people who have less experience with that particular drug. But the negative impact of so-called recreational drug use on the mental functioning of regular, experienced users is less clear, at least in the scientific literature. So I was seeking to determine the detri-mental cognitive effects of marijuana in people who smoked the drug nearly every day. I wanted to know how they would perform on mental tests after smoking, to establish whether the drug would produce widespread brain dysfunction, even if only temporarily. John was a typical participant. He smoked multiple joints nearly every day. He was affable, bright, curious, college-educated, and ambitious. He was an artist, an actor between gigs. As a result, he had the time to complete my three-session, outpatient weed study, which paid a couple of hundred dollars. Neither he nor our other research participants fit stereotypical media portrayals of the pot- head, who does little besides sitting on the couch, eating Cheetos, and playing video games. Throughout the experiment, even when under the influence of cannabis, John was lucid and socially appropriate, as were the other participants. No participant failed to show up because they had for- gotten the time or day of their scheduled appointment. Not one quit the study because the tests were too difficult or tedious. No one complained that the pot was too weak. And absolutely none of the participants ever became violent. They all complied with our stringent study rules, which imposed demands on their schedules, requiring participants to do considerable planning, to inhibit behaviors that might have been inconsistent with meeting study schedule requirements (e.g., drug use other than marijuana), and to delay immediate gratification. At the time, I didn't even register the impressive level of responsibility demonstrated by these research participants. I think, despite my best efforts, I mainly saw them as "potheads," "stoners," and all the terms that were inconsistent with the phrase "responsible adults." But I would soon enough find, working with all types of drug users throughout my career, that they have been some of the most responsible people I have ever known. "Where do you get the pot from?" John asked as he handed the roach back to me. He looked pleasantly surprised to learn that the marijuana he had just smoked was supplied by the federal government. In fact, there is only one pot supplier for researchers in the United States: the NIDA-funded University of Mississippi marijuana-cultivation program. With a huge smile plastered across his face and a twinkle in his eyes, he said, "Damn, never before have I been so proud of my government." We both laughed, but the joke also took me to a serious place. No one I knew had ever uttered the word "proud" when discussing the U.S. government and pot. Consider the fact that the federal government currently lists marijuana on Schedule I under the federal Controlled Substances Act. This means that the drug is viewed as having "no acceptable medical use in treatment" and is therefore banned in the United States, apart from limited research studies. This classification is hypocritical, although I only recently came to this conclusion. A plethora of data now demonstrates the medical utility of marijuana. We know--based on research from dozens of scientists, myself included--for example, that the drug stimulates appetite in HIV-positive patients, which could be a lifesaver for someone suffering from AIDS wasting syndrome, and that marijuana is useful in the treatment of neuropathic pain, chronic pain, and spasticity due to multiple sclerosis. The list of conditions for which marijuana has been found to be helpful grows each year. Therapeutic benefits such as these have compelled citizens to vote repeatedly over the past two decades to legalize medical marijuana at the state level. Today, thirty-three states, plus the territories of Guam and Puerto Rico and the District of Columbia, allow patients to use marijuana for specific medical conditions. In addition, since 1976, the government has supplied pot to a select group of patients, as part of their medical treatment, through the federal medical-marijuana program. And yet federal law still technically for- bids the use of marijuana for medical purposes. The inconsistency of federal laws with these initiatives and programs, and with the in- creasing number of studies demonstrating the medical usefulness of the substance, highlights our government's hypocrisy and undoubtedly undermines people's trust in the government when it comes to regulating other drugs as well. Not only has the trust of governmental regulatory agencies been eroded as a result of their handling of specific drugs, a growing number of people have begun to question the objectivity of government- funded scientists who study drugs. Consider frequent statements made by some of these scientists, including Dr. Nora Volkow, director of NIDA, which emphasize the possible neurological and psychiatric dangers of drug use--cannabis included--while virtually ignoring these drugs' potential medicinal or other beneficial effects. Nora and other scientists have been quick to caution that pot, for example, is a "gateway" drug to harder substances, but they never mention the more than half a million Americans who are arrested each year mainly for simple possession of the drug, to say nothing of the shameful racial discrimination in marijuana arrests. At the state level, black people are four times more likely to be arrested for marijuana than their white counterparts. At the federal level, Hispanics represent three-fourths of the individuals arrested for marijuana violations. This is despite the fact that blacks, Hispanics, and whites all use the drug at similar rates, and they all tend to purchase the drug from individuals within their racial groups. I would learn later in my career that the marijuana gateway theory grossly overstates the evidence by confusing correlation with causation. It's true that most cocaine and heroin users started out using marijuana first. But the vast majority of pot smokers never go on to use so-called harder drugs. To say marijuana is a "gateway" to "harder" drugs is baseless: correlation, a mere link between factors, does not mean that one factor is the cause of another. I myself had long been guilty of focusing almost exclusively on the harmful effects produced by drugs, including marijuana. In the above experiment, for example, I didn't even consider the fact that cannabis might not produce negative effects on mental performance, let alone that it might produce positive ones. In June 2000, I was invited to give a talk at a meeting of the Behavioral Pharmacology Society. My study had not yet been completed, but preliminary data were showing that the drug produced virtually no disruptive effects on the complex mental abilities (e.g., reasoning and abstraction) of regular users and that it even improved performance on a test of vigilance. And in terms of mood, the drug produced euphoria and pleasurable feelings. Never mind: at the conclusion of my talk, I virtually ignored any beneficial effects and speculated that perhaps had I given participants multiple marijuana cigarettes prior to testing their mental functioning, I would have observed more cognitive disruption. Dr. Jack Bergman, a Harvard Medical School-based psychobiologist, asked me a reasonable follow-up question: "Is it possible that marijuana, at euphoric doses, is without effect on cognitive flexibility, mental calculation, and reasoning, at least in this group of subjects?" I was so utterly focused on the harmful effects of drugs that I couldn't see this as a possibility, even though it was exactly what the data were showing. Stumped, I managed to babble on about the possibility of including more complicated test measures in future studies. Jack's question would continue to nag at me. More and more, I came to realize that drug-abuse scientists, especially government- funded ones, focus almost exclusively on the detrimental effects of drugs, even though these are, in fact, a minority of effects. This has had a damning impact on how so-called recreational drugs are regulated and inevitably on your own decision as to whether or not to partake of them. Here's the bottom line: over my more than twenty-five-year career, I have discovered that most drug-use scenarios cause little or no harm and that some responsible drug-use scenarios are actually beneficial for human health and functioning. Even "recreational" drugs can and do improve day-to-day living. Several large research studies have shown that moderate alcohol consumption, for example, is associated with decreased risk of stroke and heart disease, the top killers in the United States each year. As you will discover, a number of beneficial effects have been observed with other drugs as well. From my own experience--the combination of my scientific work and my personal drug use, I have learned that recreational drugs can be used safely to enhance many vital human activities. Excerpted from Drug Use for Grown-Ups: Chasing Liberty in the Land of Fear by Carl L. Hart All rights reserved by the original copyright owners. Excerpts are provided for display purposes only and may not be reproduced, reprinted or distributed without the written permission of the publisher.
Large Cover Image
Trade Reviews
Publishers Weekly Review
In this comprehensive and illuminating study, Columbia University psychology professor Hart (High Price) combines his scientific research on how recreational drugs impact brain functioning with reflections on his own use of heroin and other drugs. Hart's appeal for more liberal drug laws is based on his professional transformation from being a "true believer" in the ideology that drugs cause brain damage to debunker of the "distorted" science behind harsh drug policies. He views the war on drugs as a means for police departments to inflate their budgets, and points out that drugs are inherently more dangerous when illegal because people don't know what they're taking (fentanyl instead of heroin, for example). He critiques data collection methods for counting overdose deaths, noting that the qualifications for "death investigators" vary widely from state to state and that the co-occurrence of alcohol in opioid overdoses is often "completely ignored," and alleges that police often use toxicology reports to justify excessive force. Hart closes the book with a call for "respectable middle-class drug users stop concealing their use" and engage in efforts to dismantle the "antidrug bureaucracy." Careful reasoning and detailed evidence buttress the book's most surprising claims, including that methamphetamine and Adderall produce "nearly identical effects" on users. This bracingly contrarian take provides much food for thought. (Jan.)
Booklist Review
By grown-ups, Columbia University psychology professor Hart, who studies the effects of psychoactive drugs in humans, here means "autonomous, responsible, well-functioning adults" who "meet their parental, occupational, and social responsibilities." In other words, the vast majority of those who consume amphetamines, marijuana, cocaine, psychedelics, even opioids, while continuing to lead productive lives, according to the author. The larger issues, says Hart, are the demonization of those pursuing personal happiness through drugs (pursuit of happiness being at the very foundation of the Declaration of Independence), society's false hierarchy that decides which drugs are "acceptable" and "not acceptable," drug enforcement that disfavors Black people (who are 3.6 times more likely to be arrested for pot than their white counterparts, while using it at about the same rate, per the ACLU), and the lack of a governmental testing program that could detect contaminants in street drugs, and thus save possibly thousands of lives annually. Every step of the way, Hart backs up his conclusions with science, showing, for example, how the actual, usually more benign, effects of a drug can often contradict the news media's negative portrayal of it. A timely, fact-based, coherent, humane counterargument to America's spectacularly failed War on Drugs.
Kirkus Review
A full-throated defense of self-aware, voluntary drug use. Hart, the first tenured African American professor of sciences at Columbia University and former psychology department head, calls himself "an unapologetic drug user." His formulary is extensive, and it is likely only because he's tenured that he so readily admits to a liking for--but not addiction to--heroin, along with an overstuffed medicine cabinet full of other substances. His book has two overarching purposes. The first is to argue that the grown-ups of his title can indeed use drugs of varying descriptions--mostly marijuana, likely, but up to and including methamphetamines and opioids--and still be responsible parents and citizens. As the author suggests, millions of grown-ups already do so, and despite all the warnings and admonitions against it, by Hart's lights even pregnant women can use a little here and there without being or producing monsters. "It doesn't matter whether the drug is alcohol, caffeine, tobacco, or weed," he writes of the last point. "Consumption of any of these substances should be taken in consultation with a health-care professional and should be limited." His second large point is that the classification of drug use and public-health and law enforcement attitudes toward it is thoroughly racialized: Crack cocaine use was considered an epidemic evil when minorities, especially Black people, were using it. However, when it developed that "most crack users were white, and most drug users bought their drugs from dealers within their own racial group," the epidemic became one of concern rather than fear. Hart's openness in admitting to the use, often by way of experimentation, of drugs ranging from MDMA to methamphetamine and hexedrone and beyond is admirable, but doubtless, his thesis that taking drugs should be a matter of private choice alone will meet with considerable resistance. Civil libertarians will find this a valuable tool for mounting arguments in defense of free choice. Copyright (c) Kirkus Reviews, used with permission.
Summary
From one of the world's foremost experts on the subject, a powerful argument that the greatest damage from drugs flows from their being illegal, and a hopeful reckoning with the possibility of their use as part of a responsible and happy life
Table of Contents
Author's Notep. xiii
Prologue: Time to Grow Upp. 1
1The War on Us: How We Got In This Messp. 15
2Get Out of the Closet: Stop Behaving Like Childrenp. 41
3Beyond the Harms of Harm Reductionp. 59
4Drug Addiction Is Not a Brain Diseasep. 85
5Amphetamines: Empathy, Energy, and Ecstasyp. 109
6Novel Psychoactive Substances: Searching for a Pure Blissp. 131
7Cannabis: Sprouting the Seeds of Freedomp. 155
8Psychedelics: We Are Onep. 177
9Cocaine: Everybody Loves the Sunshinep. 195
10Dope Science: The Truth about Opioidsp. 221
Epilogue: The Journeyp. 245
Appendix: Death Investigation Systems by Statep. 256
Acknowledgmentsp. 265
Notesp. 267
Indexp. 283
Librarian's View
Displaying 1 of 1